Can it be ethical to implant false memories?


"I WILL break up my father's empire." The film Inception tells the story of an attempt to influence a businessman's behaviour by using futuristic gadgetry to get inside his mind.


That makes for an exciting movie, but there's a simpler way. Psychologist Elizabeth Loftus can condition people just by planting false information on a personality profile. For example, "reminding" someone of that time they drank far too much vodka as a teen may make them want to drink less of it in the future (see "I could have sworn… Why you can't trust your memory"), even if the event never occurred.


Is this ethical? That, of course, depends on how voluntary the procedure is. (Loftus has already had requests from people seeking to kick habits.) Fiddling with one's memory might seem creepy, but it has a precedent. People have long drunk to forget. Perhaps soon we'll remember to not drink.


This article appeared in print under the headline "Remember to forget"


Issue 2931 of New Scientist magazine


  • New Scientist

  • Not just a website!

  • Subscribe to New Scientist and get:

  • New Scientist magazine delivered every week

  • Unlimited online access to articles from over 500 back issues

  • Subscribe Now and Save




If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Double blasts may have birthed exotic quark stars


FORGET supernovae. Something more exotic and elusive may have been spotted – quark-novae.


When a very massive star runs out of fuel, it can explode in a supernova. The blast sometimes leaves a dense stellar remnant made mostly of neutrons.


But neutrons are made of even smaller particles called quarks. In theory, the core of a neutron star can get dense enough to undergo an additional explosive transition to create a star made mostly of quarks. Such an object would offer clues to how matter behaves at extremely high densities.


Rachid Ouyed of the University of Calgary in Alberta, Canada, and colleagues say the best evidence yet for quark stars lies with blasts called SN 2009ip and SN 2010mc. Both had two brightness peaks, unusual for supernovae. Intriguingly, SN 2009ip was labelled a supernova imposter because it flared up periodically, before finally exploding in a double-peaked burst.


Ouyed and colleagues instead think the first peak in both events was a normal supernova (arxiv.org abs/1308.3927v1). The blasts left massive, rapidly spinning neutron stars, which, as their spin slowed, got so dense that their neutrons became quarks, triggering quark-novae.


This article appeared in print under the headline "Double bangs may be quark star births"


Issue 2932 of New Scientist magazine


  • Subscribe to New Scientist and you'll get:

  • New Scientist magazine delivered every week

  • Unlimited access to all New Scientist online content -

    a benefit only available to subscribers

  • Great savings from the normal price

  • Subscribe now!




If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Swift treatment halves early death risk in HIV babies


TIME is of the essence. Giving HIV-positive infants prompt treatment halves their risk of dying early in childhood. The finding builds on results earlier this year showing that a baby and 14 adults were effectively "cured" by early treatment.


In 2005, Mark Cotton of Stellenbosch University in Cape Town, South Africa, began a study of 377 South African infants aged 6 to 12 months. One-third received no antiretroviral drugs until their immune system showed signs of weakening, in line with standard practice at the time. The others received the drugs at 7 weeks of age, on average. Of these, half were treated for 40 weeks, the others for 96 weeks.


Five years later, 21 "delayers" had died, compared with 11 and nine in the two "early" groups. What's more, 24 of the infants who had 40 weeks of treatment and 40 that received 96 weeks remain off medication (The Lancet, DOI: 10.1016/S0140 6736(13)61409-9).


Current guidelines say babies can have antiretrovirals at 6 weeks of age, but most HIV-positive babies start treatment at 6 months, Cotton says. He hopes the findings will help change that.


This article appeared in print under the headline "Swift treatment helps tots with HIV"


Issue 2932 of New Scientist magazine


  • New Scientist

  • Not just a website!

  • Subscribe to New Scientist and get:

  • New Scientist magazine delivered every week

  • Unlimited online access to articles from over 500 back issues

  • Subscribe Now and Save




If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Whales tan too, basking in the big blue


Whales tan too. Just like us, they do it to protect themselves from powerful ultraviolet radiation.


A study of skin samples taken from blue, fin and sperm whales in Mexico's Gulf of California found that the blue whales show the most obvious tanning effects. Samples were collected between January and June from 2007 to 2009.


Blue whales migrate annually between the Arctic and the Gulf of California. The team, led by Mark Birch-Machin of Newcastle University, UK, found that their grey skins darken with melanin as ultraviolet radiation intensifies from February to May in the Gulf.


Birch-Machin and his colleagues also found that damage to mitochondrial DNA in skin cells dropped as the whales' skin darkened, demonstrating that melanin protects them against UV-induced DNA damage, just as it does in humans.


Perma-tanned


But neither fin nor sperm whales appeared to change colour noticeably. Unlike blue whales, which migrate annually between the Arctic and Mexico, fin whales live in the Gulf of California all year round, and so are permanently exposed to high UV levels. They had the darkest skin with the highest melanin content.


Sperm whales spend long periods at the sea surface. For sun-factor protection, they rely on melanin and a substance called heat-shock protein 70, which repairs proteins damaged by UV light. "The amount of UV they're exposed to would overwhelm pigments alone," says Birch-Machin.


He says the results are the first to show from analysis of DNA and skin that animals rely on pigments too for sun protection. Karina Acevedo-Whitehouse of the Zoological Society of London, reported the first sighting of sunburnt whales three years ago . Birch-Machin says that whether sunburn leads to melanomas in whales – as has been documented in trout – is not known.


Journal reference: Nature Scientific Reports, DOI: 10.1038/srep02386


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Today on New Scientist


Spouse's voice easy to home in on... and easy to ignore

The brain has an uncanny ability to focus on one voice in a sea of chatter. Research on married couples shows that it may be down to familiarity of voice


Walking shark moves with ping-pong paddle finsMovie Camera

A new species of epaulette shark, discovered in Indonesia, moves across the ocean floor like a salamander – is this how the first land animals walked?


Steep rise in drug harm – opioids the most deadly

Death and disability caused by both illicit drug use and mental health disorders has climbed significantly since 1990, finds biggest epidemiological survey


Butterfly-wing electronics converts light to heat

Carbon nanotube networks fused with butterfly wings could be used to power microscopic photovoltaic cells or even to replicate DNA sequences


Out of the shadows: Picking up hints of dark matter

With more than a dozen experiments looking for it and some good theory to guide them, dark matter's days of obscurity may be numbered


I could have sworn… Why you can't trust your memory

From repressed memories to faulty eyewitness testimony, psychologist Elizabeth Loftus has revealed just how fallible our memories really are


Planet Earth was blue long before we knew

Earth may have become a watery world just 200 million years after it formed, making it a potential home for life hundreds of million years earlier than thought


Feedback: Ghost in the latrine

The internet of things, strange road crossings, vegetarian razor blades, and more


Poverty can sap people's ability to think clearly

Some poor people make poorer decisions because financial worry and all that goes with it reduces their mental bandwidth


Firms unite to bring internet to billions of new users

Mark Zuckerberg's lofty ambition of bringing connectivity to the "next 5 billion" people is backed by big companies, but will it get off the ground?


Bio-inspired speaker uses clear gel to play musicMovie Camera

The stretchy speaker is the first to use ions in place of electronics – it could be used to build noise-cancelling windows or music-making smartphone screens


Reroute town's traffic to get emergency vehicles through

Using a network of vehicle sensors – and "evolutionary algorithms" – to rejig traffic could one day get emergency responders on the scene faster


Astrophile: Milky Way's black hole is a picky eater

Our galaxy's central black hole is a fussbudget, refusing to eat most of what it pulls to its lips because the food is too hot


Whooping cranes learn migration from wise elders

Endangered whooping cranes learn way home by following older, wiser leader and don't just rely on inner GPS


Vast canyon discovered under Greenland ice sheetMovie Camera

At almost twice the length of the Grand Canyon, the enormous channel was a gushing river before Greenland froze over


Why your brain may work like a dictionary

A new analysis of the links between definitions of English words has uncovered structures that may resemble how our brains represent language


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Spouse's voice easy to home in on… and easy to ignore


Familiarity may breed contempt, and it also makes it easier to ignore our nearest and dearest.


The human brain has an uncanny ability to focus on one voice in a sea of chatterSpeaker, for example, at a party, but exactly how it does so is still up for debate.


"In the past, people have looked at the acoustic characteristics that enable the brain to do this," says Ingrid Johnsrude at Queen's University in Kingston, Ontario, Canada. "Things like differences in voice pitch or its timbre."


Johnsrude and her colleagues wondered if the familiarity of the voice also plays a role. Can people focus on one voice in a crowd more effectively if it belongs to a close relation? And is a familiar voice more easily ignored if we want to listen to someone else?


To find out, the team recruited 23 married couples. Each had been married and living together for at least 18 years.


Individuals were played two sentences simultaneously and asked to report back details about one of them, such as the colour and number mentioned. They did this correctly 80 per cent of the time when their spouse spoke the target sentence and a stranger spoke the decoy sentence. If strangers spoke both, the success rate dropped to 65 per cent.


However, if the target sentence was spoken by a stranger, and the decoy by their spouse, the success rate was 73 per cent. Taken together, the results suggest a familiar voice is easier to focus on than a stranger's – and also easier to ignore.


Or at least that's the case when you are younger. The study included married couples in their sixties and seventies as well as couples in their forties, so the team were able to assess how age affected the results. They found older people were less able to ignore their spouse's voice if they spoke the decoy sentence than younger people. There was no age difference when the spouse spoke the target sentence.


This fits with what we know about speech perception in older people, says Brittan Barker at Louisiana State University in Baton Rouge. "It's especially challenging for people who are ageing to listen to and understand speech in really noisy environments," she says. "That can result in overall declines in communication and subsequently their quality of life."


The new results imply that this inability to understand is not simply down to a loss of hearing, since older people are as capable as younger people when focusing on their spouse's voice. Instead, it likely reflects other cognitive issues, she says, such as which sensory information is prioritised. That means it may ultimately be possible to develop training techniques to combat the problem, says Barker.


Journal reference: Psychological Science, DOI: 10.1177/0956797613482467


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Spouse's voice easy to hone in on... and easy to ignore


Familiarity may breed contempt, and it also makes it easier to ignore our nearest and dearest.


The human brain has an uncanny ability to focus on one voice in a sea of chatterSpeaker, for example, at a party, but exactly how it does so is still up for debate.


"In the past, people have looked at the acoustic characteristics that enable the brain to do this," says Ingrid Johnsrude at Queen's University in Kingston, Ontario, Canada. "Things like differences in voice pitch or its timbre."


Johnsrude and her colleagues wondered if the familiarity of the voice also plays a role. Can people focus on one voice in a crowd more effectively if it belongs to a close relation? And is a familiar voice more easily ignored if we want to listen to someone else?


To find out, the team recruited 23 married couples. Each had been married and living together for at least 18 years.


Individuals were played two sentences simultaneously and asked to report back details about one of them, such as the colour and number mentioned. They did this correctly 80 per cent of the time when their spouse spoke the target sentence and a stranger spoke the decoy sentence. If strangers spoke both, the success rate dropped to 65 per cent.


However, if the target sentence was spoken by a stranger, and the decoy by their spouse, the success rate was 73 per cent. Taken together, the results suggest a familiar voice is easier to focus on than a stranger's – and also easier to ignore.


Or at least that's the case when you are younger. The study included married couples in their sixties and seventies as well as couples in their forties, so the team were able to assess how age affected the results. They found older people were less able to ignore their spouse's voice if they spoke the decoy sentence than younger people. There was no age difference when the spouse spoke the target sentence.


This fits with what we know about speech perception in older people, says Brittan Barker at Louisiana State University in Baton Rouge. "It's especially challenging for people who are ageing to listen to and understand speech in really noisy environments," she says. "That can result in overall declines in communication and subsequently their quality of life."


The new results imply that this inability to understand is not simply down to a loss of hearing, since older people are as capable as younger people when focusing on their spouse's voice. Instead, it likely reflects other cognitive issues, she says, such as which sensory information is prioritised. That means it may ultimately be possible to develop training techniques to combat the problem, says Barker.


Journal reference: Psychological Science, DOI: 10.1177/0956797613482467


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Walking shark moves with ping-pong paddle fins



A new species of walking shark has been spotted hobbling along the ocean floor off the coast of Halmahera island in Indonesia. Named Hemiscyllium halmahera after the island, the epaulette shark takes steps with its paddle-like fins, resulting in a peculiar, wriggling gait.


The clumsy footwork, similar to that of salamanders, could be a clue as to how the first land animals got about when they crawled out of the prehistoric seas. Researchers from the ReefQuest Centre for Shark Research in Vancouver, Canada, studied the gait of other epaulette sharks as a probable model for the movement of those early vertebrates.


(Image: © Conservation International/photo by Mark Erdmann)


Other researchers are more dubious. Rainer Froese from the GEOMAR Helmholtz Centre for Ocean Research Kiel, Germany, points out that epaulette sharks' gait contrasts with the movement of coelacanths. These fish are hailed as living fossils because they have hardly evolved for millions of years and their fins are thought to be closely related to the limbs of land-dwellers. "The coelacanth shows no walking behaviour at all," says Froese. H. halmahera, on the other hand, "definitely is not a living fossil but one of the 'modern' sharks," he says.


"There are several other species of weird fish that also seem to walk, such as frogfish," he adds.


The shark's walking could be an adaptation to life in coral reefs. There it is useful to be able to creep into crevices for shelter or to find food, says Luis Lucifora from the National University of Misiones in Argentina. The animals typically stroll around at night to forage for small fish and invertebrates, he says.


Researchers from Conservation International, based in Arlington, Virginia, and the Western Australian Museum in Welshpool, made the new find, which is the ninth species of walking shark to be discovered.


Journal reference: Aqua International Journal of Ichthyology, vol 19, p 123


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Steep rise in drug harm – opioids the most deadly


Regular cannabis users shaved 7000 years of healthy life off their cumulative lifespan in 2010. This may sound like a lot but it's just a drop in the ocean compared with the 9.2 million years opioid users lost, given that – at 16 million – 10 times fewer people use opioids than cannabis worldwide.


This is just one finding from the biggest epidemiological study of mental and substance-use disorders ever conducted, updating and significantly extending the first survey 20 years ago. The study, published in The Lancet, found that the burden caused by both mental and substance abuse disorders has increased significantly since 1990 but in the case of mental disorders, this was mainly the result of a growing population rather than any epidemic of poor mental health.


The results of the study should provide policy makers around the world with solid data on which to base health systems, says Louisa Degenhardt at the University of New South Wales in Australia, who led the part of the report looking at drug use.


Illicit drug use


Overall, the burden – defined as the number of healthy years of life lost because of illness, disability or early death – caused by illicit drug use increased by 52 per cent since the data was last collected. It jumped from 9.7 million years in 1990 to 17.2 million years in 2010. Worringly, about half of that increase was down to greater drug use, with population growth and an increase in the number of young people in developing countries accounting for the rest.


Cannabis is by far the most commonly used illicit drug in the world but accounts for an "absolutely tiny" health impact, says Degenhardt.


Even the modest disability caused by cannabis could be an exaggeration. The study assumed cannabis caused two disabling effects, both related to schizophrenia: causing it to start earlier and for its worst symptoms to last longer. But in an accompanying editorial, Michael Lynskey and John Strang from King's College London said that even these attributions might be regarded as controversial.


Opioids are the biggest cause of death and disability, accounting for half the illicit-drug burden, but it's also the drug class with the most effective measures to reduce its harm. Needle exchange programmes, opioid replacement therapy and HIV antiretroviral therapy could avert much of the damage, but sometimes communities are reluctant to embrace these, says Degenhardt.


A harder problem to deal with is cocaine and amphetamine use, for which proven treatments are more difficult to come by, the study authors note.


To some drug experts, the results show that current drug policies are failing. "This paper shows the cost of the dogged retention of the failed policy of drug prohibition," says Alex Wodak, a drug and alcohol researcher from St Vincent's Hospital in Sydney, Australia. "Why has the global implementation of needle syringe programmes and opioid substitution treatment been so unimpressive? Because they have been vehemently resisted by the supporters of global drug prohibition."


Mental health


The study also looked at the burden from mental disorders, which took into account those induced by substance use. It found that mental disorders are, as they were in 1990, the leading cause of non-fatal illness in the world. Back then, depression was the leading single cause of disability – now it is lower back pain.


Overall, the burden they cause is 37 per cent higher than it was in 1990, but most of that is down to a growing and greying population than a higher prevalence of mental illness per se. Harvey Whiteford from the University of Queensland, who led this part of the study, says that even if one perceives that life has become more stressful in the last 20 years, "there's no evidence that this is translating to a true increase in mental disorders".


Generally, rich countries had higher burdens than the global average because of their older populations. Countries embroiled in conflict also experienced higher than average burdens, with Afghanistan faring the worst of all. "Massive civil conflict like in Afghanistan and some African countries acts like an infectious disease – driving up prevalence," says Whiteford.


Only China, North Korea, Japan and Nigeria had burdens that were statistically lower than the global average. Whiteford suspects this might be a result of cultural differences in the way mental illness is expressed. "We are concerned that we have underestimated it for those countries because the Western instruments translated into those cultures fail to detect anxiety and depression," he says.


More resources will be needed to address mental disorders as life expectancy around the world rises, the authors conclude, since more people will be living for longer with such conditions. This will especially be the case in developing countries as the lifespans of their citizens increase. Some currently spend less than 2 per cent of their health budgets on mental health, compared with the 11 per cent spent in the UK or the 8 per cent in Australia.


But a big difference could be made at little cost. In sub-Saharan Africa, for example, the authors estimate the burden of depression could be reduced by between 10 per cent and 30 per cent for about $3 per head per year. "Primary care in developing countries is not expensive," says Whiteford. "Off-patent antidepressants are very cheap."


Journal reference: The Lancet, DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61530-5; 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Butterfly-wing electronics converts light to heat


The future of nanoscale electronics might be found on the back of a butterfly. A team led by Eijiro Miyako from the National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology used the patterns on the surface of Morpho sulkowskyi butterfly wings as a template to build carbon nanotube networks that can convert light to heat and replicate DNA sequences.


But their creation isn't just inspired by nature. It is a real hybrid of butterfly wings fused with nanocarbon that imitates traits found in nature but is also tough to reproduce through technology alone. It could potentially play a role in digital diagnosis of disease, power flexible microscopic photovoltaic cells or even help create soft wearable electronics.


The surface of Morpho wings are essentially covered in nanoscale solar cells, honeycomb-like structures that trap light, much like a fibre-optic cable, and convert it to heat to keep the insect warm in cold environments. Miyako deposited carbon nanotubes onto the butterfly wings, where they self-assembled into nanostructures that mimic the Morpho's multilayered hexagonal microstructures.


The resulting hybrid gives the term "bio-tech" new meaning: the natural pattern provided by the wings creates a large light-receiving surface area, and the physical properties of nanocarbons produce heat through vibrational energy. Lab tests confirmed that the nanotubes generate heat when struck with a laser, and Miyako says the composite material heats faster than its two components would by themselves.


It also exhibits high electrical conductivity and can also be used to make it easier to replicate DNA. Morpho wings contain layers of scales that make their surface superhydrophobic and self-cleaning. Miyako exploited this feature to initiate a method of DNA replication where drops of enzyme solution are laser-heated on the nanotube hybrid's surface. The nanocarbon network stops the material from absorbing the DNA, while the wing's water-shedding ability moves the drop away from the laser so that the process can be rapidly repeated.


"The carbon nanotube biocomposite will be very flexible and light even if it's scaled up," Miyako says.


Scaling up, however, will be rather complicated. "Maybe we should feed a lot of butterflies in factories or something like that. What do you think?" says Miyako.


Journal reference: ACS Nano, DOI: 10.1021/nn403083v


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Out of the shadows: Picking up hints of dark matter


PICK a word to describe dark matter. Mysterious? Elusive? Invisible? One you're not likely to use is "found". But after 80 years of hunting for it, that may be about to change. Several experiments deep underground have recently seen signs of something that might – just might – be dark matter. In space, too, detectors are tracking radiation that could signal the very same dark particles colliding and annihilating in our galaxy. Is it just coincidence, or could these faint fingerprints really all be from the same dark hand?


Explore our interactive map: "Around the world with dark matter"


Dan Hooper, a theoretical astrophysicist at the University of Chicago, believes that we may already have glimpsed dark matter. "I happen to be in the relatively rare minority of my colleagues who think we probably have," he says. "I'm not certain – I just think it's likely." If ...


To continue reading this article, subscribe to receive access to all of newscientist.com, including 20 years of archive content.



I could have sworn… Why you can't trust your memory


Editorial: "Can it be ethical to implant false memories?"


From repressed memories to faulty eye-witness testimony, psychologist Elizabeth Loftus has made her name working on false memory. She tells Alison George how recollections can be conjured up, and how this process could even be used in therapy


You study the fallibility of memories. Are we all prone to making things up?

We all have memories that are malleable and susceptible to being contaminated or supplemented in some way.


I hear you collect accounts of false memories.

Yes, mostly embarrassing mistakes that politicians have made. For example, Mitt Romney had a memory of being at the Golden Jubilee – an important festival in Michigan – and it turned out that the event occurred nine months before he was born.


How does this happen? What exactly is going on when we retrieve a memory?

When we remember something, we're taking bits and pieces ...


To continue reading this article, subscribe to receive access to all of newscientist.com, including 20 years of archive content.



Planet Earth was blue long before we knew


EARTH may have become the blue planet just 200 million years after it formed, making it a welcoming home for life hundreds of millions of years earlier than we thought.


Earth's first 600 million years are called the Hadean – for good reason. "It is traditionally seen as a period of Earth history when our hot, young planet was hellish and uninhabitable," says Judith Coggon at the University of Bonn, Germany.


But hell may actually have been relatively short-lived. Coggon and her colleagues have found that rocks in Greenland contain a chemical signature from the mantle 4.1 billion years ago – just 400 million years after our planet was born. That signal suggests conditions at the time may have been more like they are today than we expected.


Models suggest some "iron-loving" metals like gold and platinum – which dissolve in molten iron – should all have sunk into the iron-rich core as it formed. Because they are relatively abundant in the mantle today, it has been suggested that meteorites and comets smashing into Earth about 3.9 billion years ago replenished the stock. This hypothesised event, known as the Late Veneer, is also thought to have given Earth most of its water, delivered as ice.


But Coggon's rock samples suggest that Earth's mantle had already been topped up with iron-loving minerals by 4.1 billion years ago, meaning the Late Veneer, and the birth of Earth's oceans, must have occurred earlier. It may actually have been before 4.3 billion years ago, says Coggon: rocks of that age discovered last year hint that, at the time they formed, Earth's mantle was already rich in iron-loving minerals (Nature Geoscience, doi.org/njz).


If so, Earth gained its oceans little more than 200 million years after it formed – which also pushes back the date for the earliest possible origin of life, says Coggon.


This article appeared in print under the headline "The planet Earth was blue long before we knew"


Issue 2932 of New Scientist magazine


  • Subscribe to New Scientist and you'll get:

  • New Scientist magazine delivered every week

  • Unlimited access to all New Scientist online content -

    a benefit only available to subscribers

  • Great savings from the normal price

  • Subscribe now!




If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Feedback: Ghost in the latrine


Feedback is our weekly column of bizarre stories, implausible advertising claims, confusing instructions and more


IS THE "Internet of Things" – the computerisation of our homes heralded by futurists – really such a good idea? What happens when essential household appliances are online and vulnerable to hackers? Imagine the consequences if someone could hack into your toilet.


This is not merely a theoretical vulnerability. The Japanese company Lixil has developed a high-tech toilet called Satis that aims to be comfortable, stylish and water-saving, with features that include a lid that raises and lowers automatically without being touched, and a self-cleaning spray. Two versions are available in the US for just $4200 and $5800.


Unfortunately, a security firm called Trustwave Holdings has found a bug in the toilet's computerised control system. It is set up to allow control using an app, via Bluetooth – widely used for hands-free operation ...


To continue reading this article, subscribe to receive access to all of newscientist.com, including 20 years of archive content.



Poverty can sap people's ability to think clearly


It's the cruel cycle of poverty. The many challenges that come with being poor can sap people's ability to think clearly, according to a new study. The findings suggest that governments should think twice before tying up social-assistance programmes in confusing red tape.


Sociologists have long known that poor people are less likely to take medications, keep appointments, or be attentive parents. "Poor people make poorer decisions. They do. The question is why," says Timothy Smeeding, director of the Institute for Research on Poverty at the University of Wisconsin-Madison. But does bad decision-making help cause poverty, or does poverty interfere with decision-making?


To explore this question, psychologist Eldar Shafir at Princeton University and his colleagues took advantage of a natural experiment. Small-scale sugar-cane farmers in Tamil Nadu in southern India receive most of their year's income all at once, shortly after the annual harvest. As a result, the same farmer can be poor before harvest and relatively rich after. And indeed, Shafir's team found that farmers had more loans, pawned more belongings, and reported more difficulty paying bills before the harvest than after.


IQ drop


The researchers visited 464 farmers in 54 villages both before and after harvest. At each visit, they gave the farmers two tests of their cognitive ability: a multiple-choice pattern-matching test, and one in which they had to declare the number of digits shown rather then their value: seeing "5 5 5" but saying "three", for example.


The farmers scored significantly lower on the tests before the harvest, when money was tight, suggesting that their worries made it harder to think clearly. In fact, worrying about money impaired the farmers' thinking almost as much as going without sleep for a full night, and was the equivalent of a 13-point drop in IQ.


Looking at the same individuals before and after they received their pay packet meant that the team controlled for other factors that likely contribute to cognitive abilities, such as family background, childhood nutrition, limited education and exposure to lead or other toxins.


Mental bandwidth


The most likely explanation for the results is that people have a limited amount of "mental bandwidth", and financial worries leave less available for other cognitive tasks, says Shafir. If so, then poor people's bad decision-making may be at least partly a result of their circumstances, not due to any intrinsic lack of intelligence, says Smeeding, who was not involved in the study.


Shafir's study is an important advance, says Ann Stevens, an economist who directs the Center for Poverty Research at the University of California, Davis. If poverty makes people think less clearly, then even small social programmes to improve their lot may let them devote more attention to staying healthy, being better parents and the like. That could bring social benefits that are not usually counted in cost-benefit analyses of welfare programmes, she says.


Limited mental bandwidth also means governments should be careful not to add too much paperwork to poor people's burden. "A typical poor citizen comes to you poor in money and poor in bandwidth," says Shafir. "When you give them a 30-page application form [for social assistance], you're putting a pretty massive charge on their bandwidth."


Journal reference: Science, DOI: 10.1126/science.1238041


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Firms unite to bring internet to billions of new users


"I BELIEVE connectivity is a human right, and that if we work together we can make it reality." These were the lofty ideals Facebook founder Mark Zuckerberg revealed on 20 August, when he declared his intention to bring internet access to "the next 5 billion people" – that is, the fraction of humanity that currently lacks it.


With tech giants like Ericsson, Nokia, Samsung and Qualcomm as partners, Zuckerberg's newly formed Internet.org consortium looks to have the corporate muscle to achieve such a monumental vision.


But the specifics, at least for the moment, are fuzzy. Zuckerberg's 10-page statement talks of three broad objectives: making apps and other software more data-efficient; devising a way to make building access to the internet a profitable enterprise in its own right; and at the same time making online access affordable for all – which effectively means "free".


Making apps more efficient is the low-hanging fruit. Data compression algorithms – like those that create zip files – are used all the time. For companies like Facebook, which pay fees to transfer vast amounts of data around the internet, the incentive to squeeze down your photos, wall posts and "likes" into a slimmer data package is clear. To this end, Facebook is already trying to reduce average data usage on its Android app from 12 Mb a day to 1 Mb.


But the bigger question remains unanswered. How do you build a vast network to reach people who, in many cases, can't afford access to running water, electricity or medical care?


"The fundamental problem is whether the infrastructure is in place. Do these countries have a 'backbone', like BT fibre, which they can use?" asks Colin Beeke, a technology specialist at the University of West London.


Partner companies could help here. Qualcomm, for example, based in San Diego, California, makes chips for wireless communication devices, and played a key role in devising the 3G and 4G standards used by cellular networks across the developed world. The company predicts data demand will grow 1000-fold over the next decade. As it focuses on meeting those needs, it could find cheaper, more efficient data transmission methods that could have a trickle-down effect in the developing world.


If that pans out, there is some evidence that content providers would be willing to make their products freely available to mobile users. Since 2010, Facebook has formed partnerships with mobile data providers in developing countries to "zero-rate" Facebook's mobile traffic, so that it doesn't count against users' paid data plans. The Wikimedia Foundation has followed suit. Its Wikipedia Zero project allows people in several developing countries, primarily in Africa and Asia, to read the Wikipedia online encyclopedia in their native language without being billed for the data.


Of course, every company wants an opportunity to grow its market, and Internet.org will have competition. Google has its own lofty plan for the developing world: Project Loon involves floating thousands of balloons about 20 kilometres up in the stratosphere and beaming down Wi-Fi signals to otherwise unconnected regions. It may sound like pie in the sky, but they are ahead of Internet.org. In a test in June, Project Loon flew 30 balloons over New Zealand, allowing a few lucky locals to get online, absolutely free.


This article appeared in print under the headline "A connected world"


Issue 2932 of New Scientist magazine


  • Subscribe to New Scientist and you'll get:

  • New Scientist magazine delivered every week

  • Unlimited access to all New Scientist online content -

    a benefit only available to subscribers

  • Great savings from the normal price

  • Subscribe now!




If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Bio-inspired speaker uses clear gel to play music



An almost-invisible film of jelly can now be hooked up to a laptop to blast out your favourite tunes. The stretchy speaker, which can produce sounds that span the entire audible range, is the first to use ions in place of electronics. The technology could one day be used to build both noise-cancelling windows and music-playing smartphone screens.


The team that created the device, led by Zhigang Suo of Harvard University, took inspiration from the way electric signals are transmitted in the human body. There, the flow of charged atoms called ions – rather than the electrons that carry charge in electrical devices – is what allows neurons to share information or trigger the heart to beat.


Suo's team combined saltwater – which is packed with dissolved ions that are free to move around – with a polymer. The result was a flexible but solid substance known as a hydrogel.


The speaker was created by sandwiching a thin sheet of transparent, insulating rubber between two layers of gel, which were both connected to copper electrodes.


Ionic music


Electrical audio signals from a computer, fed to the hydrogel via the electrodes, caused the ions in both layers to flow. That physical movement then caused the insulating rubber in the middle to vibrate at specific frequencies, producing different sounds.


The team foresees many potential applications. One is as a coating for TV, laptop or smartphone screens that doubles up as a speaker. It might also be used to create soundproof windows, using the film to generate noise-cancelling vibrations.


In contrast with other bendy electronics, the material can be stretched more than five times its length without an increase in resistivity, a property that might allow other applications to be dreamed up.


Hydrogels are inexpensive and easy to make but the problem is they don't last long. They dry out as their water evaporates. In order to develop longer-lasting devices, Suo is seeking alternative, flexible ionic conductors that do not readily evaporate. "The current work is really a proof of concept," he says.


Journal reference: Science 10.1126/science.1240228


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Reroute town's traffic to get emergency vehicles through


Ambulances could get to the scene of traffic accidents twice as fast if cities decide to adopt emerging intelligent transportation systems (ITS) technology. So say researchers in Spain who have used algorithms that mimic evolution to reduce response times for emergency vehicles. The faster response could save lives, the team says, with very little impact on overall traffic speeds.


ITS involves using both vehicle-to-vehicle and vehicle-to-infrastucture links to improve both road safety and traffic flow. The idea is to use signals from Wi-Fi routers mounted along roads to communicate with vehicles and keep track of their location. Combined with communications between cars, this could help avoid collisions, for instance, by sending signals that automatically apply brakes if a crash is imminent. And traffic jams could be alleviated by the infrastructure's smart use of traffic-light phasing.


While such scenarios remain in the future for now – most vehicles and cities aren't yet equipped with such technology – Francisco Martinez and colleagues at the University of Zaragoza in Spain wondered what effect ITS systems could have on emergency vehicle response times. If they can alleviate traffic jams, they reasoned, how much quicker could police, paramedics and firefighters get through to the scene of an incident?


Clear a path


To find out, they simulated how ITS infrastructure software might be set up in New York City, San Francisco and Rome, using mapping data for those cities from the open source Open Street Map. They then analysed how traffic built up when an accident blocked a road and nearby junctions, and developed a host of software routines designed to get emergency vehicles to the scene the fastest.


They found that their best solution used a genetic algorithm – which mimics the process of natural selection to evolve the fittest solution to a problem – to work out the best route for the emergency vehicle to take. At the same time, it searched for the best way to reroute other traffic to give the emergency vehicle the room on the roads that it needed.


They found that an ITS system using their software could reduce the emergency services' arrival time by an average of 48 per cent, and with a 14 per cent hike in travel time for the civilian vehicles that were rerouted. The researchers say their evolutionary software could be vital in ensuring car-accident victims are on the operating table within the "golden hour" – the time after which surgical intervention has a markedly lower chance of saving lives.


It would certainly appear to offer a leap above what most emergency services enjoy today. "We don't have the ability to control the traffic lights – we rely on the lights and sirens on our vehicles to get through traffic," says Jenny Round of the London Ambulance Service.


Journal reference: Expert Systems and Applications, doi.org/nmv


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.




Astrophile: Milky Way's black hole is a picky eater


Astrophile is our weekly column on curious cosmic objects, from the solar system to the far reaches of the multiverse


Object: the Milky Way's black hole


Food source: big, gassy stars


Our galaxy's central black hole is a fussbudget, refusing to eat most of what it pulls to its lips because the food is too hot. Spitting out its meals may not only stunt its growth – the finicky black hole may also be preventing new stars from being born nearby.


Most large galaxies like the Milky Way are thought to harbour black holes at their cores that have a mass equal to that of millions or billions of suns. Some of these behemoths are enthusiastic eaters, pulling in surrounding gas with abandon. As the gas falls towards the black holes, it heats up, producing blazing beacons known as quasars that can be seen across the universe.


The Milky Way's relatively dim black hole, known as Sagittarius A* (pronounced "a-star"), is decidedly not one of these gorgersMovie Camera. A crowded disc of massive stars spins around it, and researchers had previously calculated that these stars should spew out enough gas in stellar winds to provide the black hole with about four Earths' worth of meals over the course of a year. But it does not seem to be swallowing that much material – if it were, it would shine 100 million times brighter in X-rays.


Scalding soup


Some scientists noted, though, that the brightness estimate assumed the gas coming from the stars was relatively cool, and that it could easily slip down the black hole's gullet, says Daniel Wang of the University of Massachusetts in Amherst. To find out what's really going on, Wang and his colleagues used NASA's Chandra X-ray Observatory to measure the temperature and brightness of gas at different distances from the black hole.


They found that the gas gets hotter and less abundant the closer it is to the black hole. The researchers estimate that less than 1 per cent of the surrounding gas ultimately comes near enough to be eaten. Wang thinks the food is just too hot.


Quasars are champion eaters because they slurp up relatively cool gas, below 1 million °C. Such gas is dense and flows in an orderly fashion into a quasar's maw, like water swirling into a drain. But the gas around Sagittarius A* is much hotter – collisions between stellar winds in the starry disc heat the gas to 10 million °C before it even starts to fall towards the black hole. This hot gas is tenuous and its particles zip around randomly, making it hard to corral.


"It's very hard to get steam into the sink," says Wang. That means the black hole should not get the blame for apparently turning up its nose at hot gas on its plate. "The black hole wants to suck it in, but it cannot," says Wang. Bizarrely, the tiny fraction of gas our black hole does imbibe may get in because it has transferred some of its jitteriness to gas particles that are thrown outwards, possibly by the black hole's own magnetic field lines.


Snack time


Superhot stellar gas ejected from the black hole might be heating up other gas clouds surrounding the galactic centre, which is bad news for star formation, because they form when gas is cool enough to condense into dense bundles. All the extra heat may be stopping star birth. "That will have an effect on the evolution of the galaxy," says Wang.


Lest anyone worry that Sagittarius A* will starve as it tries, and mostly fails, to sip up the scalding soup of stellar gas, it may soon get a cooler snack as massive as three EarthsMovie Camera.


"In the next few months, a large cloud of gas is on course to collide with the black hole," says Jeremy Schnittman of NASA's Goddard Space Flight Center in Greenbelt, Maryland, in an article accompanying the new study. Astronomers will be watching intently to see if Sagittarius A* opens wide.


Journal reference: Science, DOI: 10.1126/science.1240755


If you would like to reuse any content from New Scientist, either in print or online, please contact the syndication department first for permission. New Scientist does not own rights to photos, but there are a variety of licensing options available for use of articles and graphics we own the copyright to.



Have your say

Only subscribers may leave comments on this article. Please log in.


Only personal subscribers may leave comments on this article


Subscribe now to comment.




All comments should respect the New Scientist House Rules. If you think a particular comment breaks these rules then please use the "Report" link in that comment to report it to us.


If you are having a technical problem posting a comment, please contact technical support.